Social Learning Theory

Social Learning Theory: Developed significantly by Albert Bandura, Social Learning Theory bridges the gap between behaviorism and cognitive theories. This subtopic focuses on the idea that individuals can learn through observation, imitation, and modeling, in addition to direct experience. It emphasizes the importance of attention, retention, reproduction, and motivation in the learning process, and highlights reciprocal determinism, where an individual's behavior, environment, and cognitive factors all interact and influence each other. This theory is particularly relevant to understanding how social contexts and interactions contribute to learning.

Understanding How Social Contexts and Interactions Contribute to Learning

Introduction to Social Learning: Beyond Direct Experience

The quest to understand how humans learn has been a central theme in psychology for centuries. For much of the early 20th century, the field was dominated by theories that emphasized direct, personal experience as the primary engine of learning. Behaviorism, championed by figures such as B.F. Skinner and Ivan Pavlov, posited that learning occurred through processes like classical and operant conditioning, where behaviors were acquired or modified based on direct interactions with the environment, such as stimulus-response associations and the immediate consequences of actions (reinforcement or punishment).1 While these theories provided valuable insights into certain types of learning, they struggled to account for the full spectrum of human learning, particularly the rapid acquisition of complex behaviors or instances where individuals learn without undergoing personal trial-and-error or receiving direct reinforcement.3 For example, behaviorism, with its focus on operant conditioning, suggested that behaviors are primarily shaped by their immediate outcomes.4 However, this model could not fully explain how individuals could learn novel behaviors quickly or why imitation and the effects of reinforcement might be delayed.4

It was within this theoretical landscape that psychologist Albert Bandura emerged as a pivotal figure. In the 1960s, Bandura proposed his Social Learning Theory (SLT), a framework that fundamentally shifted the understanding of how learning occurs.1 He argued that individuals learn a significant amount by observing others within social contexts.3 According to Bandura, “most human behavior is learned observationally through modeling: from observing others one forms an idea of how new behaviors are performed, and on later occasions, this coded information serves as a guide for action”.6 This perspective did not entirely dismiss the role of reinforcement, but it critically added social and cognitive dimensions to the learning process, positioning SLT as a vital bridge between behaviorist and cognitive learning theories.3

The core premise of SLT is that learning is not solely an individualistic endeavor confined to direct experience. Instead, it is profoundly intertwined with social interactions and the observation of others—their actions, attitudes, and the consequences that follow.3 A crucial aspect of Bandura’s theory is that learning can occur simply by observing another’s behavior, even if the observer does not immediately exhibit a change in their own behavior.3 This is vital because it explains how individuals can acquire new, complex patterns of behavior by watching others, forming a mental representation of how these behaviors are performed, which then serves as a blueprint for future actions.6

The emergence of SLT represented a significant challenge to the prevailing behaviorist view that learning must involve direct experience and reinforcement. Behaviorist theories, often relying on animal studies or highly controlled experiments, focused on individual responses to environmental stimuli.2 Bandura, however, observed that this model was insufficient to explain the full range of human learning, especially the rapid acquisition of novel behaviors or learning that occurred without personal trial-and-error.3 His theory introduced a mechanism—observational learning—that explained how complex behaviors could be learned efficiently, without every individual needing to undergo the same laborious conditioning process. This acknowledged a more advanced and adaptive learning capability in humans, suggesting that we do not always need to experience consequences firsthand to learn from them.

Furthermore, SLT’s emphasis on the “social context” as a primary learning environment fundamentally altered the understanding of where and how learning takes place. While cognitive theories focused on internal mental processes, they did not always place the social environment at the forefront as the primary stage for these processes to unfold in relation to learning new behaviors.1 SLT explicitly states that we learn “from interacting with others in a social context”.3 The environment, in this view, is not merely a source of stimuli but a rich source of modeled behavior and observed consequences. This recognition expanded the scope of what constitutes an “educational” experience, implicating everyday interactions, cultural norms, and media exposure as critical arenas for learning.

The Pillars of Social Learning: How We Learn from Others

Albert Bandura’s Social Learning Theory is built upon several key pillars that explain the mechanisms through which individuals learn from their social environment. These concepts illuminate how observation, cognitive processing, and reciprocal interactions shape human behavior and knowledge acquisition.

Observational Learning: The Power of Watching and Modeling

At the heart of SLT is observational learning, the process by which individuals learn by watching the actions of others (referred to as models) and the consequences that follow those actions.3 This powerful mechanism allows for the acquisition of new skills, knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors without the need for direct, personal experience.6 Models can take various forms:

  • Live Models: These are actual individuals demonstrating a behavior in person.8 Examples include a teacher illustrating how to solve a mathematical equation, a parent preparing a meal, or a coach demonstrating a sports technique.
  • Symbolic Models: These encompass real or fictional characters who display behaviors through various media such as books, films, television programs, or online platforms.8 A character in a novel exhibiting courage, an athlete in a commercial showcasing a product, or an influencer on social media demonstrating a particular skill are all examples of symbolic models. The influence of symbolic models has become particularly potent in the digital age, where media exposure is pervasive.16
  • Verbal Instructional Models: This type of modeling involves learning through descriptions and explanations of a behavior, rather than direct visual observation of the action itself.15 Following written instructions to assemble furniture, listening to a podcast that explains a scientific concept, or adhering to a recipe are instances of learning from verbal instructional models.

Imitation is the direct action of using someone or something as a model and copying their behavior.8 Whether an observed behavior is imitated depends on several factors, including the characteristics of the model (e.g., perceived similarity to the observer, status, attractiveness), the observer’s perceived ability to perform the behavior (self-efficacy), and the observed consequences of the model’s behavior.3

Closely related to imitation is identification, a form of influence where an individual adopts an attitude or behavior because they want to be associated with a particular person or group (the model).8 The more an observer relates to or feels similar to a model, the more likely they are to imitate them, often because they expect to experience similar outcomes in comparable situations.8

The introduction of symbolic models by Bandura radically expanded the scope of social learning beyond the immediate physical presence of a model. This acknowledges that learning can occur from individuals one has never met and even from fictional representations. In our modern, media-saturated world, individuals are constantly exposed to a vast and diverse array of symbolic models through television, movies, the internet, and social media. These models can shape attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors on a mass scale, often without the observer critically evaluating the source or the implications of the modeled behavior.8 This has profound implications for child development, the transmission of cultural norms, and public health initiatives.

The Four Mediational Processes: The Cognitive Journey from Observation to Action

Bandura proposed that learning through observation is not a simple, automatic process of mimicry. Instead, he argued that internal cognitive processes mediate between the environmental stimulus (the observed behavior) and the individual’s response (imitation or performance of the behavior).3 These mediational processes determine whether a new response is actually acquired and, if so, whether it is performed. The four key mediational processes are:

  1. Attention: For learning to occur, the observer must first pay attention to the model and accurately perceive the significant features of the modeled behavior.3 Several factors can influence attention, including the characteristics of the model (e.g., if the model is perceived as interesting, attractive, prestigious, or similar to the observer) and the distinctiveness of the behavior (e.g., if it is striking, novel, or particularly relevant to the observer’s needs or goals).3 Observer characteristics, such as their motivation, interests, cognitive capabilities, and pre-existing knowledge, also play a crucial role.5 Distractions or lack of interest can impair attention and, consequently, learning.
  2. Retention: Once attention is secured, the observed behavior must be remembered or retained in memory.3 This involves encoding the information about the behavior and storing it, often in the form of mental images (visual representations) or verbal descriptions (a series of instructions).4 Rehearsal, both mental and physical, can significantly enhance the retention of observed behaviors.3
  3. Reproduction (Motor Reproduction): After attending to and retaining the information, the observer must possess the physical and cognitive capabilities to replicate the observed behavior.3 This stage involves translating the stored mental representations into overt actions. The ability to reproduce a behavior depends on factors such as the observer’s motor skills, physical strength, and developmental readiness.3 For instance, a young child might observe an adult performing a complex gymnastic move (attention and retention) but lack the physical development to reproduce it accurately.
  4. Motivation: Finally, even if a behavior has been observed, remembered, and the observer is capable of reproducing it, they must be motivated to actually perform the behavior.3 Learned behaviors are not always demonstrated. Motivation is influenced by several factors:
  • Vicarious Reinforcement/Punishment: This is a critical concept in SLT. Observing others being rewarded (vicarious reinforcement) or punished (vicarious punishment) for their behavior significantly influences the observer’s motivation to imitate that behavior.4 If a model is seen to receive praise, rewards, or achieve a desired outcome, the observer is more likely to imitate the behavior. Conversely, if the model is punished or experiences negative consequences, imitation is less likely. This form of reinforcement is indirect, as the observer learns from the model’s experiences rather than their own.3 This is a highly efficient learning mechanism, allowing individuals to learn from the successes and mistakes of others without having to personally undergo the risks or costs associated with direct experience, which has significant adaptive value for survival and success.13
  • Self-Efficacy: An individual’s belief in their own capability to successfully perform the observed behavior is a powerful motivator.5 People with high self-efficacy for a particular task are more likely to attempt it, expend greater effort, and persist longer in the face of difficulties or setbacks.5 Conversely, low self-efficacy can deter an individual from attempting a behavior, even if they have learned it. Self-efficacy acts as a critical filter and energizer within the mediational process. While often directly linked to the motivation and reproduction stages, very low self-efficacy for a task might lead an individual to avoid it altogether 22, potentially meaning they do not even pay attention to models performing that task or make an effort to retain the information, deeming it irrelevant or beyond their capabilities.
  • Expectation of Future Reinforcement: The anticipation of receiving rewards or positive outcomes for performing a behavior can be as motivating, if not more so, than experiencing the reward itself.4 This expectation influences cognitive processes, including attention and the overall drive to learn and perform the behavior.

These mediational processes underscore that SLT is not merely a theory of “monkey see, monkey do.” It posits a complex cognitive journey from observation to action. A crucial implication of these processes is the distinction between learning and performance. An individual can learn a behavior (i.e., attend to it and retain it in memory) but may not perform it if they lack the physical ability (reproduction) or the necessary motivation.3 This explains phenomena like latent learning, where knowledge or skills are acquired but not demonstrated until there is an incentive or need to do so.

The following table summarizes these mediational processes:

Table 1: The Four Mediational Processes in Observational Learning

ProcessDescriptionKey Influencing FactorsExample
AttentionThe extent to which the observer notices and focuses on the model’s behavior.Model characteristics (e.g., similarity, prestige, attractiveness, novelty of behavior 3), observer characteristics (e.g., interest, motivation, perceptual set, cognitive capabilities, distraction levels 5).A child pays closer attention to a cartoon character (symbolic model) they admire performing an action.
RetentionThe ability to remember the observed behavior over time.Symbolic coding, mental images, verbal descriptions 4, cognitive organization, rehearsal (mental or physical 3).An apprentice mentally rehearses the steps a master craftsperson demonstrated earlier in the day.
ReproductionThe ability to physically and cognitively replicate the observed behavior.Physical capabilities, motor skills 5, cognitive skills, developmental readiness 3, self-observation of reproduction, accuracy of feedback.A student attempts to replicate a complex dance move after watching an instructor perform it.
MotivationThe will or desire to perform the learned behavior.Vicarious reinforcement/punishment 8, direct reinforcement, self-reinforcement, self-efficacy 22, outcome expectancies 4, perceived value of the behavior.An employee decides to adopt a new sales technique after seeing a colleague achieve high sales and receive a bonus using it.

Reciprocal Determinism

Bandura also introduced the concept of reciprocal determinism to explain the complex and continuous interplay between an individual’s behavior (B), personal factors (P – including cognitions, beliefs, personality, and biological characteristics), and environmental influences (E).4 According to this principle, these three factors are not unidirectional causes but rather interact with and influence each other dynamically. For example, a child’s belief in their academic abilities (P) can influence their study habits (B), which in turn can affect their academic performance and the feedback they receive from teachers (E). This feedback then further shapes their beliefs about their abilities (P), creating an ongoing cycle. This model highlights the active role individuals play in shaping their own experiences and development.

The Bobo Doll Experiments: Seeing is Believing (and Doing)

Perhaps the most iconic empirical support for Social Learning Theory comes from Albert Bandura’s series of “Bobo Doll” experiments conducted in the early 1960s.24 These studies were designed to demonstrate that learning, particularly of aggressive behavior, could occur through observation without the need for direct reinforcement, a direct challenge to prevailing behaviorist theories which often attributed aggression solely to frustration or direct conditioning processes.25

Methodology Overview

The classic experimental setup typically involved preschool-aged children as subjects.25 These children were generally divided into three main groups:

  1. One group observed an adult model behaving aggressively towards an inflatable toy clown called a “Bobo doll.”
  2. Another group observed an adult model behaving non-aggressively, often playing quietly with other toys and ignoring the Bobo doll.
  3. A control group was not exposed to any adult model.

The adult models demonstrated specific behaviors. In the aggressive condition, the model would physically attack the Bobo doll (e.g., punching it, kicking it, hitting it with a mallet, throwing it in the air) and also exhibit verbal aggression (e.g., shouting aggressive phrases like “Sock him in the nose,” “Hit him down,” or “Pow”).25 In the non-aggressive condition, the model would play with other toys, such as a Tinkertoy set, and ignore the Bobo doll.25

Following exposure to the model, the children were typically subjected to a mild “aggression arousal” phase. For instance, they might be taken to a room with attractive toys but then told they could not play with them, inducing a degree of frustration.25 Finally, each child was individually taken to another experimental room that contained various toys, including the Bobo doll, a mallet, and other items that could be used for aggressive or non-aggressive play. Researchers then observed the children’s behavior through a one-way mirror for a set period, typically 20 minutes, and rated the degree of physical and verbal aggression they displayed.25

Key Findings

The results of the Bobo Doll experiments were striking and provided strong support for Bandura’s hypotheses:

  • Imitation of Aggression: Children who observed the aggressive adult model exhibited significantly more aggressive behaviors towards the Bobo doll compared to children in the non-aggressive model group or the control group.25 Crucially, these children often mimicked the specific aggressive actions and verbalizations they had witnessed from the adult model. This demonstrated not just a general increase in aggression but the imitation of novel aggressive acts. This specificity was revolutionary because prior theories might have predicted only a general increase in arousal or disinhibition of existing aggressive responses. The fact that children replicated unique aggressive acts implied a detailed cognitive encoding (retention) and subsequent reproduction of the observed behaviors, supporting the role of mediational processes.
  • Generalization of Aggression: Exposure to aggressive models also led to an increase in overall aggressive behavior, not limited to direct imitation of the model’s actions.25
  • Influence of Symbolic Models: Variations of the experiment demonstrated that children also imitated aggressive behaviors viewed on videotape, indicating that symbolic models (characters or actions depicted in media) could be as influential as live models in promoting observational learning.25
  • Vicarious Reinforcement: Later variations of the Bobo Doll studies explored the impact of observed consequences. Children who saw the adult model being rewarded for their aggressive behavior were more likely to exhibit aggression themselves. Conversely, children who observed the model being punished for aggression displayed significantly less aggression.19 This finding was pivotal, as it provided a causal link between observed consequences and the likelihood of behavioral performance, thereby solidifying the “motivation” aspect of the mediational processes. It showed that learning could be separated from performance, and that vicarious reinforcement played a key role in translating learned behaviors into action.
  • Gender Differences: Some gender-related patterns were observed. For example, male children were somewhat more prone to imitate physical aggression—a behavior often typed as masculine in many cultures—than female children. However, there were generally no significant differences in the imitation of verbal aggression, which is less sex-typed.25 Additionally, children tended to imitate same-sex models more readily for certain types of behavior.25

Significance and Implications

The Bobo Doll experiments had profound implications for psychology and our understanding of learning:

  • They provided robust empirical evidence for the concept of observational learning, demonstrating that individuals can acquire new behaviors simply by watching others, without direct instruction or personal reinforcement.25
  • The studies powerfully illustrated that reinforcement does not have to be direct; vicarious reinforcement (and punishment) significantly influences whether a learned behavior is performed.8
  • These experiments ignited a long-standing and important debate about the impact of media violence on children’s behavior and development, a discussion that remains highly relevant today.13
  • The findings challenged the Freudian notion of catharsis, which suggested that viewing violence could allow individuals to vicariously release aggressive drives. Bandura’s evidence indicated the opposite: observing aggression tends to increase, rather than decrease, aggressive behavior.25

Interestingly, the ethical concerns raised by critics regarding the Bobo Doll experiments—specifically, the potential harm of exposing children to aggressive models 26—inadvertently highlight the very potency of observational learning that Bandura aimed to demonstrate. The concern itself is rooted in the acceptance of the theory’s core tenet: that observing behavior, even within an experimental context, can have a tangible and potentially lasting impact on the observer. If there were no perceived risk of learning and imitation, the ethical objections would carry far less weight. Thus, the debate surrounding the ethics of the experiments serves as an unintentional testament to the power of the learning mechanisms Bandura identified.

The Evolution and Expansion: From Social Learning to Social Cognitive Theory

Albert Bandura’s theoretical framework did not remain static. Over time, he refined and expanded his ideas, leading to a significant evolution from Social Learning Theory (SLT) to what became known as Social Cognitive Theory (SCT). This transition, formally marked by his 1986 publication “Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory,” reflected a deeper appreciation for the role of cognitive processes in human learning and agency.27

The Naming Shift: Why “Cognitive”?

The change in nomenclature from “Social Learning Theory” to “Social Cognitive Theory” was a deliberate and meaningful one. Bandura made this alteration for two primary reasons:

  1. To Emphasize Cognition: The foremost reason was to underscore the critical and central role that cognitive processes—such as thinking, self-reflection, self-regulation, information encoding, and belief systems—play in human learning, motivation, and functioning.27 Bandura wanted to move beyond a view of individuals as passive recipients of environmental influences, instead highlighting their capacity as active agents who interpret experiences, construct reality, and guide their own actions through thought.
  2. To Distance from Other Theories: The shift also served to differentiate his evolving theory from other prevalent “social learning theories” of that era.27 Some of these earlier theories, like the one proposed by Miller and Dollard in 1941 which was based on drive reduction principles 27, did not accord such a prominent role to cognition or might have retained interpretations closer to behaviorism. Bandura’s SCT offered a more nuanced and comprehensive perspective.

Deepening the Cognitive Dimension

Social Cognitive Theory significantly expanded upon the foundations of SLT by placing greater emphasis on internal cognitive factors that influence how individuals learn and behave. Key among these deepened cognitive dimensions are:

  • Self-Efficacy: This concept is a cornerstone of SCT and represents one of Bandura’s most significant contributions.5 Self-efficacy is defined as an individual’s belief in their capabilities to organize and execute the courses of action required to attain designated types of performances.22 It is crucial to note that self-efficacy is not about the actual skills one possesses, but rather the belief in one’s ability to use those skills effectively in specific situations.22 These beliefs profoundly influence an individual’s choice of activities (people tend to engage in tasks where they feel competent and avoid those where they do not), the amount of effort they expend, their persistence in the face of difficulties or failures, and their emotional reactions (e.g., stress, anxiety).22 High self-efficacy generally leads to greater effort, resilience, and ultimately, higher levels of achievement. Bandura argued that self-efficacy beliefs are a key component of human agency, enabling individuals to exercise control over their lives.22 This concept provides a crucial psychological mechanism explaining why some individuals exposed to the same models and social environments learn and perform differently, introducing a powerful individual difference variable.
  • Self-Regulation: SCT emphasizes the capacity of individuals to control their own thoughts, feelings, and actions, a process known as self-regulation.4 This is not an innate trait but a set of learned skills involving several sub-functions:
  • Goal Setting: Establishing personal objectives and standards for performance.
  • Self-Observation (Self-Monitoring): Paying attention to one’s own behavior, the conditions under which it occurs, and its effects.
  • Self-Judgment (Self-Evaluation): Comparing one’s performance to personal standards or external benchmarks.
  • Self-Reaction: Administering self-rewards (e.g., pride, satisfaction) for meeting standards or self-punishments (e.g., dissatisfaction, self-criticism) for falling short. Through these self-regulatory processes, individuals can direct their own behavior, motivate themselves, and make changes, becoming less reliant on external rewards and punishments to guide their actions.4
  • Human Agency: SCT portrays individuals not merely as passive products of their environment or internal drives, but as proactive agents who actively contribute to their own development, shape their environments, and influence their life paths.27 This agentic perspective includes:
  • Forethought Capability: The ability to anticipate the likely consequences of actions, set goals for the future, and plan strategies to achieve them.26
  • Symbolic Capability: The use of symbols, particularly language and mental imagery, to process information, extract meaning from experiences, solve problems cognitively, engage in reflective thought, and communicate effectively across time and space.27
  • Reciprocal Determinism (Revisited and Emphasized): While the concept of reciprocal influences was present in SLT, SCT provided a more elaborate and central role for triadic reciprocal determinism. This refers to the continuous, dynamic, and mutual influence exerted between three sets of factors: Personal factors (including cognitions, emotions, self-beliefs, and biological attributes), overt Behavior, and the external Environment.4 These interactions are bidirectional; for example, the environment influences behavior, but behavior also changes the environment, and personal factors like thoughts and beliefs shape how the environment is perceived and acted upon. This dynamic interplay suggests that interventions aimed at behavior change can target any of these three factors, as a modification in one will inevitably ripple through and affect the others, offering multiple leverage points for practical applications in various fields.

Distinctions from Contemporary Theories

SCT clearly distinguished itself from other prominent psychological theories of its time:

  • It fundamentally differed from behaviorist theories by robustly emphasizing internal cognitive processes and self-beliefs as causal factors in behavior, rather than viewing them as mere byproducts or irrelevant “black box” phenomena.27 Behaviorists largely ignored such self-processes, focusing on how external stimuli directly caused behavior.
  • SCT also offered a more nuanced view than theories that overemphasized biological determinism. While acknowledging the influence of evolutionary factors and biological endowments, Bandura stressed the unique human capacity for creating complex social and technological innovations, which in turn create new environmental selection pressures that influence human adaptation and even biological evolution over time.27 This highlighted a bidirectional relationship between biology and culture.

The evolution from SLT to SCT signifies a significant theoretical advancement, moving towards a more holistic and agentic view of the learner. It is not just about how social learning occurs, but also about the learner’s active role in initiating, directing, and sustaining their own learning and behavior through the powerful mechanisms of self-belief, self-regulation, and proactive engagement with their world.

Social Learning in Action: Applications Across Diverse Contexts

The principles of Social Learning Theory (SLT) and its evolution into Social Cognitive Theory (SCT) have demonstrated remarkable versatility and practical utility across a wide array of human endeavors. The core concepts of observational learning, modeling, mediational processes, vicarious reinforcement, self-efficacy, and reciprocal determinism provide a robust framework for understanding and influencing behavior in diverse settings. The pervasiveness of these applications underscores the fundamental nature of learning from others in social contexts as a deeply ingrained human characteristic.

In Education: Shaping Learning Environments and Instruction

Educational settings are rich environments for social learning. Teachers and peers serve as constant models, shaping not only academic skills but also attitudes, values, and social competencies.

  • Teacher Modeling: Teachers are powerful live models. They demonstrate problem-solving strategies, critical thinking skills, enthusiasm for subjects, and classroom behavior.1 Students observe these displays and may imitate them, influencing their own approach to learning and interaction.
  • Peer Influence and Collaborative Learning: Students learn extensively by observing and interacting with their peers.19
  • Peer Tutoring: In peer tutoring arrangements, the tutor reinforces their own understanding by explaining concepts (modeling effective explanation), while the tutee benefits from relatable peer instruction and observes successful learning strategies.31 Both parties engage in observational learning and modeling.
  • Cooperative Learning Groups: When students work together on tasks or projects, they observe diverse approaches to problem-solving, share responsibilities, and learn crucial teamwork and communication skills through interaction and mutual modeling.30 These settings can also align with Vygotsky’s concept of the Zone of Proximal Development, where peers scaffold each other’s learning.32
  • Classroom Role-Playing: This technique allows students to act out scenarios related to lesson content (e.g., historical events, social dilemmas). It helps them understand different perspectives, practice social behaviors, and apply knowledge in a controlled environment, observing and modeling various responses.31
  • Flipped Classroom Model: In this model, students often watch instructional videos (symbolic models) at home and then engage in activities in the classroom, applying what they’ve learned. This classroom time frequently involves observing peers and receiving guidance from the teacher, further facilitating social learning.19
  • Building Self-Efficacy: Educators can foster students’ self-efficacy by providing positive reinforcement for effort and achievement, offering constructive feedback, structuring tasks to allow for mastery experiences, and exposing them to successful peer models.8
  • Curriculum Design: Educational materials can be thoughtfully designed to include diverse and positive role models, helping students from various backgrounds to identify with successful individuals and see possibilities for themselves.29

In Psychology and Therapy: Understanding and Modifying Behavior

SLT/SCT principles are foundational to many therapeutic approaches aimed at understanding and changing maladaptive behaviors and promoting psychological well-being.

  • Behavior Modeling for Phobias: Stemming directly from Bandura’s early work, therapists may model fearless interaction with a phobic stimulus (e.g., handling a snake for someone with a snake phobia), or clients may observe live or filmed models who have successfully overcome similar fears.23 This observational learning helps the client acquire a new, non-anxious response and increases their self-efficacy for confronting the feared object or situation.
  • Skill Acquisition: Therapists often model desired social skills, communication techniques, assertiveness, coping mechanisms, or anger management strategies.23 Clients then have the opportunity to practice (reproduce) these skills, typically with feedback from the therapist. This approach is effective in treating a range of issues, including anxiety disorders, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), substance use disorders, and difficulties with emotional regulation.23
  • Cognitive Restructuring: While primarily a cognitive therapy technique, SLT principles can support it. Clients can learn more adaptive thought patterns and self-talk by observing these modeled by the therapist or by peers in group therapy settings.33
  • Developing Healthier Networks: A direct application of SLT involves encouraging individuals to surround themselves with positive role models who exhibit the behaviors and lifestyles they wish to adopt (e.g., associating with sober peers during addiction recovery, or joining groups focused on healthy eating and exercise).33

In the Workplace: Enhancing Training, Leadership, and Culture

Organizations increasingly leverage social learning principles to improve employee performance, develop leaders, and foster positive workplace cultures.

  • Employee Training and Onboarding: New employees often learn job responsibilities, technical skills, company procedures, and unwritten cultural norms by observing experienced colleagues (a process often formalized as shadowing) and through mentorship programs where senior employees act as models and guides.34
  • Leadership Development: Aspiring leaders can develop their capabilities by observing the behaviors, decision-making styles, and communication strategies of effective current leaders and mentors. Executive coaching frequently incorporates modeling of leadership skills and provides feedback based on observed performance.34
  • Fostering a Learning Culture: Organizations can promote a continuous learning environment by encouraging knowledge sharing through internal forums, communities of practice, and peer-to-peer learning initiatives. When leaders visibly model a commitment to their own learning and development, it encourages others to do the same.16
  • Workshops and Role-Playing: These are commonly used for training skills such as customer service, negotiation, conflict resolution, or ethical decision-making, allowing employees to observe modeled best practices and then practice these behaviors in simulated work scenarios.34

In Public Health: Promoting Healthy Behaviors and Training Professionals

SLT/SCT provides a valuable framework for public health interventions aimed at promoting healthy lifestyles and preventing illness.

  • Health Promotion Campaigns: Public health campaigns often use respected public figures, celebrities, or relatable peers as symbolic models to advocate for healthy behaviors (e.g., anti-smoking campaigns, safe sex promotion, encouraging physical activity or healthy eating).36 These campaigns frequently highlight the positive outcomes (vicarious reinforcement) experienced by those who adopt the desired behaviors.
  • Training Healthcare Professionals: The principles of SLT are effectively used in training programs for nurses, nursing assistants, and other healthcare providers. Such programs emphasize observational learning of clinical procedures from experienced professionals, enactive learning (hands-on practice with feedback), and behavioral reinforcement to improve competencies in areas like patient care, safety protocols, and disease management (e.g., pressure injury prevention).37
  • Addressing Deviant Behavior: SLT helps explain how negative or risky behaviors, such as substance abuse or aggression, can be learned through observation of peers, family members, or media portrayals.33 Interventions can then focus on providing exposure to positive role models, teaching alternative coping strategies, and modifying the social environment to support healthier choices.

The Influence of Social Circles: Family, Peers, and Community

The immediate social environment plays a critical role in shaping learning and development from a very early age.

  • Family: Parents, siblings, and other caregivers are primary and highly influential models for children. Through consistent observation and imitation, children learn a vast range of behaviors, attitudes, emotional responses, communication styles, social norms, and problem-solving skills.1 The consequences of behaviors observed within the family (e.g., praise for sharing, disapproval for aggression) act as powerful vicarious reinforcers.
  • Peer Groups: Especially during childhood and adolescence, peers become increasingly significant models. Observation of peers’ behaviors—including academic engagement, social interactions, risk-taking, and fashion choices—and the associated social rewards or punishments (e.g., acceptance, popularity, disapproval) profoundly shapes an individual’s own choices, values, and self-efficacy.1
  • Community: Broader community norms, cultural values, and the visibility of local role models (e.g., community leaders, successful professionals, local heroes) also contribute to social learning by providing examples of accepted behaviors and aspirational pathways.

The Digital Age: Learning Through Media, Social Media, Online Communities, and Immersive Technologies

The advent of digital technologies has exponentially expanded the reach and nature of social learning.

  • Traditional Media (TV, Films): Symbolic models presented in television programs and films have long been recognized for their capacity to influence viewers’ attitudes and behaviors, both positively (e.g., inspiring prosocial actions, raising awareness about social issues) and negatively (e.g., normalizing violence, perpetuating stereotypes).16
  • Social Media Platforms (Facebook, Instagram, TikTok, X, etc.): These platforms have become powerful engines for social learning. Users are constantly exposed to a deluge of content from peers, influencers, and organizations, leading to the observation and imitation of trends, opinions, skills, and lifestyles. Vicarious reinforcement, in the form of likes, shares, comments, and follower counts, is immediate, highly visible, and can strongly motivate imitation.16 Social media facilitates rapid information (and misinformation) dissemination and can significantly shape identity formation as users curate online personas and are influenced by perceived peer norms and public figures.40
  • Online Learning Platforms and Communities (MOOCs, Forums): Digital learning environments, including Massive Open Online Courses (MOOCs), online forums, and collaborative workspaces, facilitate social learning through features like discussion boards, peer feedback mechanisms, collaborative projects, and the observation of others’ contributions, questions, and problem-solving approaches.14
  • MOOCs: While MOOCs offer vast opportunities for accessing information and can incorporate social learning tools, they also present challenges related to learner motivation, self-regulation, and satisfaction. Research indicates that SLT concepts like self-efficacy and self-regulated learning skills are significant predictors of student success and their ability to overcome barriers in MOOC environments.43 Evidence suggests that students who actively engage in collaborative learning features within MOOCs tend to perform better than those who learn in isolation.43 However, the effectiveness of these platforms often hinges on both their design and the learner’s own agency and skills in leveraging social learning opportunities. The “social” component isn’t an automatic benefit in a digital space; it requires active fostering and engagement.
  • Virtual Reality (VR) and Augmented Reality (AR): These emerging immersive technologies offer novel and powerful environments for observational learning and modeling.
  • VR and AR can provide highly realistic simulations for training in complex, high-stakes, or dangerous tasks (e.g., surgical procedures, flight simulation, emergency response, equipment repair), allowing learners to observe expert models and practice skills in a safe, controlled, and repeatable manner.15
  • These technologies can enhance engagement, motivation, and knowledge retention by enabling “learning by doing” in response to observed models or digitally overlaid guided instructions.45
  • VR, in particular, can assist learners in locating relevant content within simulated environments, connecting with other learners or experts in shared virtual spaces, and fostering motivation through engaging experiences.46 Applications are being explored in metaverse-based learning, transforming social media interactions, therapeutic interventions, diverse training programs, and distance education.46
  • Despite their potential, challenges associated with VR/AR include issues of accessibility and cost, the need for technical expertise in content development, user adoption and comfort (e.g., motion sickness), and significant ethical considerations related to data privacy and the potential for misuse or misinformation.45

The effectiveness of SLT-based interventions often depends on the quality and relatability of the chosen model and the clarity of the observed consequences. Simply exposing an individual to a model is insufficient. If the model is not perceived as credible, competent, or similar enough to the observer, or if the outcomes of their behavior are ambiguous or irrelevant, learning and imitation are less likely to occur.3 This implies careful consideration in designing interventions, whether in education, therapy, or public health campaigns.

Furthermore, the rise of digital and social media presents both a monumental opportunity and a considerable challenge for social learning. While these platforms democratize access to an unprecedented number of models and vast amounts of information, they also create an environment where negative, harmful, or unverified behaviors and beliefs can be rapidly learned and disseminated.16 This underscores a growing need for critical media literacy skills, enabling individuals to evaluate the credibility of online models and the potential consequences of imitating observed behaviors.

The following table provides a snapshot of SLT’s diverse applications:

Table 2: Illustrative Applications of Social Learning Theory

DomainSpecific Application ExampleKey SLT/SCT Principle(s) Illustrated
EducationPeer tutoring where a student explains a concept to a classmate.Observational learning (of teaching and learning strategies), modeling, vicarious reinforcement (from peer success), self-efficacy. 31
TherapyBehavior modeling for treating a specific phobia (e.g., fear of flying).Observational learning (of a model calmly interacting with feared stimulus), vicarious reinforcement (model shows no harm), increased self-efficacy, motivation. 23
WorkplaceA new sales employee shadows a top-performing senior sales representative during client calls.Observational learning (of sales techniques, communication skills), modeling (of professional behavior), retention (of procedures), motivation (from seeing success). 34
Public HealthAn anti-bullying campaign featuring popular teen influencers sharing personal stories and promoting kindness.Symbolic modeling, identification, vicarious reinforcement (influencers receive positive feedback), attention (due to influencer status). 36
Digital MediaAn individual learns a new software program by watching a series of YouTube tutorial videos.Symbolic modeling, attention (to clear demonstrations), retention (of steps), reproduction (practicing the skill), motivation (to achieve desired outcome). 16
ParentingA child learns to say “please” and “thank you” after consistently observing their parents using these phrases and receiving positive responses.Observational learning, modeling (of polite behavior), vicarious reinforcement (observing positive social outcomes), retention and reproduction. 10

Critical Perspectives: Strengths, Limitations, and Its Place Among Learning Theories

Albert Bandura’s Social Learning Theory (SLT) and its later evolution into Social Cognitive Theory (SCT) have profoundly influenced the field of psychology, offering a more nuanced understanding of how humans learn and develop. However, like all influential theories, SLT/SCT is not without its critics and limitations. A balanced perspective requires acknowledging both its significant contributions and the areas where it faces challenges or requires further refinement.

Strengths of Social Learning Theory

SLT/SCT possesses several key strengths that account for its enduring impact:

  • Comprehensive and Explanatory Power: SLT provides a more holistic explanation of learning than pure behaviorism by integrating cognitive factors, social context, and behavioral principles. It can account for a wide range of learned behaviors, including complex social skills, attitudes, values, and emotional reactions, which are difficult to explain solely through direct conditioning.3
  • Empirical Support: The core tenets of SLT/SCT, particularly concepts like observational learning, vicarious reinforcement, and self-efficacy, are backed by a substantial body of empirical research, most famously Bandura’s Bobo Doll experiments.17
  • Practical Applications: The theory has demonstrated immense practical utility, leading to effective interventions and strategies in diverse fields such as education (e.g., cooperative learning, teacher modeling), therapy (e.g., behavior modeling for phobias, skills training), organizational development (e.g., mentoring, leadership training), public health (e.g., health promotion campaigns), and parenting.17
  • Flexibility: SLT is flexible enough to explain variations in an individual’s behavior or learning process when their environment changes. It also acknowledges that learning can occur through multiple pathways, including direct experience and observation.47
  • Highlights Cognitive Processes: A major strength is its explicit recognition of internal cognitive (mediational) processes such as attention, retention, reproduction, and motivation, effectively bridging the gap between behaviorist and cognitive learning theories.3
  • Emphasis on Self-Efficacy and Agency: The evolution to SCT, with its strong emphasis on self-efficacy and human agency, provided invaluable insights into the role of individuals’ beliefs about their capabilities and their capacity to actively shape their own development and environment.22 This offered a more optimistic and empowering view of human potential compared to more deterministic theories.

The strengths of SLT, particularly its successful integration of cognitive and social factors with behavioral principles, along with its clear practical applicability, are precisely why it has had such a lasting and widespread impact. It offered a more nuanced and realistic view of human learning than its predecessors, filling a significant gap in psychological theory. Its “bridging” nature was key to its success—it adopted useful elements from behaviorism (like the importance of reinforcement) and combined them with critical cognitive insights and the undeniable power of social observation.3

Limitations and Criticisms of Social Learning Theory

Despite its strengths, SLT/SCT has faced several criticisms:

  • Underemphasis on Biological Factors: A common critique is that the theory does not adequately address the influence of biological, genetic, or hormonal factors on behavior and learning.16 It tends to lean more heavily on environmental and cognitive explanations (nurture) with less focus on innate predispositions (nature).
  • Oversimplification of Learning: Some critics argue that SLT/SCT may oversimplify the true complexity of human learning. It might not fully account for the intricate developmental changes that occur across the lifespan or the full depth of internal cognitive processing involved in understanding and meaning-making.17 For instance, the theory does not extensively detail how specific behaviors or the capacity for certain types of learning change as an individual matures.17
  • Limited Ecological Validity of Early Experiments: The foundational Bobo Doll experiments, while groundbreaking, were conducted in controlled laboratory settings. This raises questions about the extent to which the findings can be generalized to more complex, real-world social situations where multiple interacting factors are at play.26
  • Ethical Concerns with Experiments: The methodology of some early experiments, particularly those involving exposing children to aggressive models, has raised ethical concerns regarding the potential for unintended negative impacts on the participants.26
  • Difficulty in Measuring Cognitive Processes: Internal cognitive processes such as attention, retention, and motivation, while central to the theory, can be inherently difficult to observe, operationalize, and measure directly and objectively.16
  • Doesn’t Account for All Behavior: SLT/SCT cannot explain all types of human behavior. For example, it may struggle to account for behaviors that emerge without any apparent observational model or instances where individuals act in ways that are contrary to the behaviors they have observed being reinforced.17
  • Downplays Personal Accountability/Choice (in some interpretations): By placing a strong emphasis on environmental influences and modeling, some critics have suggested that the theory might inadvertently downplay individual responsibility or free will. However, the robust emphasis on human agency, self-regulation, and self-efficacy within SCT largely counters this interpretation by highlighting individuals’ capacity for self-direction.17
  • Cultural Bias: Much of the foundational research for SLT/SCT was conducted with participants from Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) societies.26 This raises concerns about the cultural generalizability of some findings, as learning processes, valued behaviors, and influential models can vary significantly across different cultural contexts.
  • Socioeconomic Factors: The theory may not sufficiently address how socioeconomic status and related factors (e.g., access to resources, quality of education, exposure to diverse role models) can influence learning opportunities and developmental trajectories.26

It is important to note that many of these criticisms, such as the underemphasis on biology or potential cultural bias, are not unique to SLT/SCT. They are common critiques leveled against many psychological theories developed within specific cultural and historical contexts, particularly those originating in the mid-20th century. This does not necessarily invalidate the core mechanisms of SLT, but it does suggest that the expression and prioritization of certain modeled behaviors, as well as the nature of who constitutes an influential model, might vary significantly across cultures and could be influenced by biological predispositions. The theory provides a fundamental framework, but its specific parameters may require adjustment and contextualization when applied to diverse populations and settings.

SLT in Dialogue with Other Theories

Understanding SLT/SCT also involves situating it within the broader landscape of learning theories.

  • Behaviorism (e.g., Skinner, Pavlov):
  • Differences: SLT posits an active learner who makes choices (e.g., whom to attend to) rather than the passive responder often depicted in behaviorism. SLT distinguishes between the acquisition of knowledge and its performance, whereas behaviorism tends to equate learning with observable performance. In SLT, reinforcement is often indirect (vicarious) and mediated by cognitive factors, unlike the direct stimulus-response-consequence chains in behaviorism. SLT research primarily involves human participants, while behaviorism often relied on animal studies.12 Behaviorism focuses on learning through conditioning; SLT adds observation and cognitive mediation as key learning mechanisms.2
  • Bridge: SLT incorporates the principle of reinforcement from behaviorism but argues that it is not the sole driver of learning and can operate indirectly.3 It moves beyond classical behaviorism by introducing crucial social and cognitive elements.3 The evolution from SLT to SCT, particularly the emphasis on self-efficacy, directly addresses some of behaviorism’s limitations by providing an internal, cognitive explanation for why the same reinforcer might not be universally effective and why individuals actively choose certain paths and persist in goal-directed behavior even without immediate external reinforcement.22
  • Cognitivism:
  • Similarities: Both SLT/SCT and cognitivism emphasize the importance of internal mental processes in learning.9 SLT is often considered a bridge to, or even a type of, cognitive theory because it explicitly includes concepts like attention, memory, and motivation.3
  • Differences: While both value cognition, SLT specifically highlights observational learning within a social context as a primary mechanism. General cognitive theories cover a broader spectrum of mental processes (e.g., information processing, language acquisition, complex problem-solving) that are not necessarily tied to direct social observation or modeling.2 SCT, with its inclusion of human agency and self-regulation, has a broader theoretical scope than the original SLT, but cognitivism as an entire field might be considered even broader in terms of the range of cognitive functions it seeks to explain.49
  • Constructivism (e.g., Piaget, Vygotsky):
  • Bandura vs. Piaget: Both theorists view the learner as active. However, Piaget’s constructivism focuses on the individual’s construction of knowledge through distinct developmental stages and direct interaction with the physical environment, involving processes of assimilation and accommodation.50 Bandura, in contrast, emphasizes learning through the observation of social models. Piaget generally believed that cognitive development precedes learning, whereas Bandura suggested that cognitive development is necessary for effective imitation.50
  • Bandura vs. Vygotsky: Both Bandura and Vygotsky underscore the importance of the social context in learning. However, Vygotsky’s Social Constructivism places a primary emphasis on learning through direct interaction, collaboration, and language with more knowledgeable others (MKOs) within the learner’s Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD).1 Learning is seen as a co-constructed process, often facilitated by scaffolding provided by the MKO. Bandura’s SLT, while acknowledging interaction, focuses more on learning through the observation and imitation of models, which can occur with less direct, reciprocal interaction (e.g., learning from symbolic models in media).
  • Key Distinction: Constructivist theories, in general, portray learners as actively building their own understanding and knowledge structures, often through personal exploration or the social negotiation of meaning.1 SLT, while acknowledging cognitive processing, focuses more on the acquisition of behaviors, knowledge, and attitudes through observing and internalizing what others do and the consequences they experience. The distinction is nuanced, particularly with Vygotsky’s Social Constructivism. While SLT emphasizes learning from models and constructivism emphasizes learners building knowledge, Vygotsky’s framework, with its concepts of the MKO and ZPD, inherently involves observational learning and modeling as integral parts of the collaborative knowledge construction process. The MKO often demonstrates skills or thought processes (modeling), which the learner observes, attempts to replicate, and then refines through interaction and feedback. Thus, rather than being entirely separate, Vygotsky’s theory can be seen as integrating observational learning as a mechanism within a broader, socially mediated constructive process.

The following table provides a comparative overview:

Table 3: Social Learning Theory vs. Other Major Learning Theories

FeatureBehaviorismSocial Learning Theory (SLT/SCT)CognitivismConstructivism (Piaget & Vygotsky)
Primary Learning MechanismConditioning (classical, operant), ReinforcementObservation, Modeling, Vicarious Reinforcement, Cognitive MediationInformation Processing, Mental Models, Schema DevelopmentActive Construction of Knowledge, Discovery (Piaget), Social Interaction & Scaffolding (Vygotsky) 1
Learner’s RolePassive Responder to environmental stimuliActive Observer, Processor of information, AgenticActive Processor of InformationActive Constructor of Meaning and Knowledge 1
Role of Social ContextSource of stimuli and reinforcersPrimary source of models, vicarious experiences, and interactionsOne of many contexts for cognitive processingEssential for Vygotsky (cultural tools, MKO); Environment for exploration for Piaget 32
Role of CognitionLargely Ignored (“Black Box”)Central: Mediational Processes (Attention, Retention, Reproduction, Motivation), Self-Efficacy, Self-Regulation 3Central: Memory, Attention, Perception, Problem-SolvingCentral: Schema Development (Piaget), Internalization of social speech, Higher Mental Functions (Vygotsky) 2
Key ProponentsPavlov, Watson, SkinnerAlbert BanduraMiller, Neisser, Atkinson, ShiffrinJean Piaget, Lev Vygotsky 1
Learning & PerformanceOften seen as the sameDistinct: Learning can occur without immediate performance 12Focus on internal cognitive change; performance is an outcomePerformance demonstrates the learner’s constructed understanding and capabilities

Conclusion: The Enduring Impact of Learning in a Social World

Albert Bandura’s Social Learning Theory, and its subsequent evolution into Social Cognitive Theory, fundamentally reshaped our understanding of how individuals learn and develop. By moving beyond the limitations of earlier theories, SLT/SCT illuminated the profound role of social contexts and interactions in shaping human behavior. Its core contributions—the concepts of observational learning, the critical role of mediational cognitive processes (attention, retention, reproduction, and motivation), the power of vicarious reinforcement, the significance of self-efficacy beliefs, and the dynamic interplay of reciprocal determinism—have provided a comprehensive and enduring framework.13 The theory uniquely bridged the gap between behaviorist perspectives, which emphasized environmental influences, and cognitive approaches, which focused on internal mental states, by demonstrating how these elements interact within a social matrix.3 This shift was not merely academic; it recognized the inherently social nature of human learning.

The principles of SLT/SCT are arguably more relevant today than ever before, particularly in our hyper-connected, media-saturated world.15 The nature of “models” and “social environments” has expanded exponentially with the rise of the internet, social media, online communities, and immersive digital technologies. Individuals are constantly exposed to a global array of symbolic models, and vicarious reinforcement occurs instantaneously through likes, shares, and comments. Understanding SLT/SCT provides critical tools for navigating this complex informational landscape, for discerning credible models, and for comprehending the subtle yet powerful ways in which online interactions shape our beliefs, attitudes, and actions.

The implications of Social Learning Theory extend broadly, impacting both individuals and society at large:

  • For Individuals: The theory offers a pathway to personal agency and empowerment. By understanding that learning can occur through observation and that self-beliefs like self-efficacy play a crucial role, individuals can become more conscious learners. They can actively choose positive role models, cultivate their self-efficacy through mastery experiences and supportive feedback, and engage in self-regulation to achieve personal goals and modify undesirable behaviors. In a world filled with external influences, especially via pervasive media, SCT’s emphasis on self-efficacy and self-regulation provides tools for individuals to critically assess observed behaviors, choose what to internalize and imitate, and strive for personal growth despite external pressures.22 This understanding can foster media literacy and personal empowerment, enabling more conscious navigation of complex social environments.
  • For Society: SLT/SCT underscores the significant responsibility held by educators, parents, media creators, community leaders, and policymakers. Recognizing the power of modeling and the impact of observed consequences necessitates a commitment to fostering environments that promote positive social learning and mitigate the influence of negative or harmful models. This has direct implications for curriculum development, parenting practices, media content guidelines, public health campaigns, and the design of social systems that encourage prosocial behavior and skill development. The core message of SLT—that we learn by observing others in a social context—is not just a psychological theory but a fundamental aspect of human culture and societal evolution. It provides a micro-level psychological process that underpins the macro-level transmission of norms, traditions, skills, and values across generations and within communities.3

The story of social learning is continually unfolding. While Bandura laid a robust foundation, the principles of SLT/SCT must be continuously re-evaluated and applied to understand emerging forms of social learning and influence, particularly as social contexts themselves are transformed by technological advancements. Ongoing research into learning within digital environments, the neural underpinnings of observational learning (such as the role of mirror neuron systems, briefly noted in some literature 11), and the cross-cultural applicability of these concepts will continue to enrich and refine this vital theory. The dynamic framework of Social Learning Theory ensures its continued relevance in explaining how we learn from, and with, each other in an ever-changing social world.

Works cited

  1. Learning Theories: Five Theories of Learning in Education | NU, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.nu.edu/blog/theories-of-learning/
  2. Learning Theory Comparison | Cheryl D. Calhoun, accessed May 13, 2025, https://cheryldcalhoun.com/2014/09/24/learning-theory-comparison/
  3. What Is Bandura’s Social Learning Theory? 3 Examples, accessed May 13, 2025, https://positivepsychology.com/social-learning-theory-bandura/
  4. Social Learning Theory | EBSCO Research Starters, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.ebsco.com/research-starters/education/social-learning-theory
  5. Social Learning Theory – Bandura, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.structural-learning.com/post/social-learning-theory-bandura
  6. How Social Learning Theory Works | People & Culture – UC Berkeley, accessed May 13, 2025, https://hr.berkeley.edu/grow/grow-your-community/wisdom-caf%C3%A9-wednesday/how-social-learning-theory-works
  7. Albert Bandura’s Social Learning Theory – isEazy, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.iseazy.com/blog/bandura-social-learning-theory/
  8. Social learning theory -A-Level Psychology – Study Mind, accessed May 13, 2025, https://studymind.co.uk/notes/social-learning-theory/
  9. (PDF) Social Learning Theory: Cognitive and Behavioral Approaches, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.researchgate.net/publication/367220348_Social_Learning_Theory_Cognitive_and_Behavioral_Approaches
  10. How Social Learning Shapes Child Development, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.scalliwagschildcare.com/blog/how-social-learning-shapes-child-development.html
  11. Social Learning Theory – The Decision Lab, accessed May 13, 2025, https://thedecisionlab.com/reference-guide/psychology/social-learning-theory
  12. Differences between Behaviourism and Social Learning Theory …, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.tutor2u.net/psychology/reference/differences-between-behaviourism-and-social-learning-theory
  13. Social Learning Theory: How Bandura’s Theory Works – Verywell Mind, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.verywellmind.com/social-learning-theory-2795074
  14. The Power of Social Learning Theory in Education – Lambda Solutions, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.lambdasolutions.net/blog/the-power-of-social-learning-theory-in-education
  15. Social Learning Theory & Its Modern Application in Education for 2025 | Research.com, accessed May 13, 2025, https://research.com/education/social-learning-theory
  16. Social Learning Theory: Key Principles and Modern Applications, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.ispringsolutions.com/blog/social-learning-theory
  17. How Social Learning Theory Works – Docebo, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.docebo.com/learning-network/blog/social-learning-theory/
  18. www.ebsco.com, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.ebsco.com/research-starters/education/social-learning-theory#:~:text=In%20its%20simplest%20form%2C%20social,influence%20the%20process%20as%20well.
  19. A Guide to Social Learning Theory in Education, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.wgu.edu/blog/guide-social-learning-theory-education2005.html
  20. SOCIAL LEARNING THEORY — APPROACHES – PsychStory, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.psychstory.co.uk/approaches/social-learning-theory
  21. studymind.co.uk, accessed May 13, 2025, https://studymind.co.uk/notes/social-learning-theory/#:~:text=Observation%20involves%20watching%20the%20behavior,ability%20to%20perform%20a%20behavior.
  22. Academic self-efficacy: from educational theory to instructional …, accessed May 13, 2025, https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC3540350/
  23. Using Behavior Modeling Therapy to Treat Phobias – Verywell Mind, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.verywellmind.com/behavior-modeling-2671528
  24. en.wikipedia.org, accessed May 13, 2025, https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Bobo_doll_experiment#:~:text=Between%201961%20and%201963%2C%20he,physically%20abusing%20the%20Bobo%20doll.
  25. Bobo doll experiment | Description, Methodology, Results, & Facts …, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.britannica.com/event/Bobo-doll-experiment
  26. Albert Bandura on Social Learning Theory, Social Cognitive Theory …, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.earlyyears.tv/albert-bandura-on-social-learning-theory-social-cognitive-theory-self-efficacy-and-bobo-doll/
  27. people.wku.edu, accessed May 13, 2025, https://people.wku.edu/richard.miller/banduratheory.pdf
  28. Social cognitive theory – Wikipedia, accessed May 13, 2025, https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Social_cognitive_theory
  29. Educational learning theories and how to apply them – University of Phoenix, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.phoenix.edu/articles/education/educational-learning-theories.html
  30. The Impact of Social Learning Theory in Education System | Free …, accessed May 13, 2025, https://aithor.com/essay-examples/the-impact-of-social-learning-theory-in-education-system
  31. A Complete Guide To Social Learning Theory (10 Use Cases …, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.classpoint.io/blog/social-learning-theory
  32. Social Learning Approaches: The Impact of Observation and …, accessed May 13, 2025, https://teachers.institute/learning-teaching/social-learning-impact-observation-interaction/
  33. Applying Social Learning Theory in Social Work & Education – Positive Psychology, accessed May 13, 2025, https://positivepsychology.com/social-learning-theory-social-work/
  34. How to apply social learning theory in the workplace – Easygenerator, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.easygenerator.com/en/blog/e-learning/social-learning-theory-how-to-apply-it-in-the-workplace/
  35. Social Learning Theory: Benefits, Examples, and Best Practices, accessed May 13, 2025, https://saima.ai/blog/social-learning-theory
  36. Discuss the Social Learning Theory in health promotion. – TutorChase, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.tutorchase.com/answers/a-level/psychology/discuss-the-social-learning-theory-in-health-promotion
  37. Development of a social learning theory-based pressure … – Frontiers, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.frontiersin.org/journals/public-health/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2024.1478147/full
  38. www.ispringsolutions.com, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.ispringsolutions.com/blog/social-learning-theory#:~:text=Social%20learning%20theory%20also%20explains,content%20might%20reinforce%20aggressive%20tendencies.
  39. Learning in the Digital Age – eSchool News, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.eschoolnews.com/digital-learning/2024/01/03/learning-in-the-digital-age/
  40. Social Learning Theory in the Age of Social Media, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.neurohealthalliance.org/post/social-learning-theory
  41. Reflecting on Social Learning Tools to Enhance the … – Frontiers, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.frontiersin.org/journals/education/articles/10.3389/feduc.2020.606533/full
  42. The Role of Social Learning Platforms in eLearning – Mindsmith, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.mindsmith.ai/blog/the-role-of-social-learning-platforms-in-elearning
  43. 10 Effective Strategies for MOOC Courses Success – Infopro Learning, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.infoprolearning.com/blog/maximizing-your-learning-experience-strategies-for-success-in-mooc-courses/
  44. ajet.org.au, accessed May 13, 2025, https://ajet.org.au/index.php/AJET/article/download/5919/1658/19951
  45. Immersive Learning and Training Using AR & VR – BrandXR, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.brandxr.io/immersive-learning-and-training-using-augmented-and-virtual-reality
  46. VR in social learning: applications and challenges | Request PDF, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.researchgate.net/publication/376140982_VR_in_social_learning_applications_and_challenges
  47. www.onlinemswprograms.com, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.onlinemswprograms.com/social-work/theories/social-learning-theory/#:~:text=One%20of%20the%20primary%20strengths,for%20different%20ways%20of%20learning.
  48. Introduction to social learning theory in social work, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.onlinemswprograms.com/social-work/theories/social-learning-theory/
  49. Difference Between Social Cognitive Theory and Social Learning …, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.tutorialspoint.com/difference-between-social-cognitive-theory-and-social-learning-theory
  50. www.reading.ac.uk, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.reading.ac.uk/education/-/media/project/uor-main/schools-departments/education/documents/offer-holders/early-years-2024-25/pg28-theories-of-child-development_article.pdf?la=en&hash=654E89A90DDE3B9779CCADFE6BF80B17
  51. Difference Between Sociocultural Theory And Constructivism – Cram, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.cram.com/essay/Difference-Between-Sociocultural-Theory-And-Constructivism/P3Y7A7LG6EE5
  52. What’s the difference between social constructivism and social constructionism? – Reddit, accessed May 13, 2025, https://www.reddit.com/r/AskSocialScience/comments/d2d4ug/whats_the_difference_between_social/
Categories: